Sight-Reading: Factors That Affect Piano Sight-Reading and Practice Methods
Essay Preview: Sight-Reading: Factors That Affect Piano Sight-Reading and Practice Methods
Report this essay
Sight-Reading: Factors that Affect Piano Sight-Reading and Practice Methods
Introduction
The definition of sight-reading is “the ability to read and perform music at first sight, i.e. without preparatory study of the piece” (Apel, 1962, p.679). Lowder (1983) surveyed college faculty members and in-service teachers to find out what they believed were the most important piano skills. The survey found that sight-reading ability was ranked second (“cadence” was first), followed by score-reading, harmonization, and accompaniment. A parallel result was found in the more recent survey of college piano students ranked sight-reading skill as the second most desirable skills (surpassed by “musicality”) (Kostka,1997).
Many music educators believe that the most important way to acquire good sight-reading skills is by reading extensively and practising it regularly, i.e. reading large amounts of music and playing many different unfamiliar music. Mursell (1956) and Elliott (1982) confirmed the validity of this belief. Although reading and practising extensively has proved to improve sight-reading, in the 20th and 21st century, a number of sight-reading researches have found other factors that can affect sight-reading achievement and have devised strategies to teach the skill of sight-reading. This essay is to discuss the factors that affect piano sight-reading, and explore practise methods to improve the performance of it.
Factors affecting sight-reading achievements and their practise method
Orientation of the keyboard
Playing music on first sight requires treating the functioning of eyes and hands separately. Since both have grasp much at the same time and within fractions of a second, they must learn to function independently. The eyes are solely directed at the images of the notes and the fingers must learn to dominate the keys blindly. This eliminates the double attention of the eyes of reading the music and looking for the keys (i.e. blind attack of the keys)—the most important premise for sight-reading (Spillman, 1990).
To achieve the skill of “blind attack of the keys”, the elevated groups of the upper keys (black keys) can help us. One should aware that the octave is divided into two groups of keys—two-black-group and three-black-group. One should first practise by closing the eyes to find the black-key-group, and after one is familiar with it, one should close eyes again and practise finding the white key in the mind (e.g. the E note). The procedure of learning to find the white key if to first find the end keys of the black-key-group (the 2 white keys at the two sides of the black-key-group), then find the middle keys (Cranmer, 1979).
Pattern Detection Skill and Memorisation
Research found that there is a positive relationship between intelligence (in the area of pattern detection skill) and music reading (Boyle, 1970; Keilmann, 1972; Ullman, 1945). Pattern detection skill is essential in sight-reading to identify patterns that construct the music. These patterns include repetition, sequences, short motives, long motives, scalic pattern, arpeggios, broken chords, alberti bass, moving in parallel, contrary motion, chromatic, 3rd apart, and more. If rhythmic patterns in the score are systemically detected, the score might not be as hard or complicated as it looks. In addition, detecting musical patterns help one to memorise the music quickly (so that one can remember what is read and able to play them while reading ahead. Or, if one has pre-studied the score, one might be able to memorise the score and do the sight-reading task with ease).
Pattern detection skills can be trained by analysing the score (finding the patterns, tonality, key changes, harmonies, rhythmic patterns, and others) before playing it, and aim to memorise the piece as soon as possible (this needs great amount of concentration and concentration can be trained as a �by-product’ too through this process) Regular training on patterns detection and regular memorisation of different kinds/styles of music will improve the sight-reading ability (Boyle, 1970; Keilmann, 1972; Ullman, 1945).
Rhythmic Patterns and Underlying Beat
Elliott (1982) supports the belief already held by many instrumental music educators that regular practice of reading rhythmic pattern is likely to improve sight-reading ability; this is particularly true for wind players. Past research found that the ability to read music at sight is dependent in a large part upon the ability to read rhythms at sight. They speculated the reason for that was because music is a �on-going’ event; pitch is important, but without the sense of duration and rhythm, pitches could not combine in a sense to make �a piece of music’ (Boyle, 1970; Elliott, 1982; Kostka, 2000; Ullman, 1945).
Underlying Beat
In order to be good at sight-reading rhythmic patterns in a piece, one has to be good at feeling the underlying beat of the music (Elliott, 1982; Wilhelm, 1972). Many research found that underlying beat is best to experience through body movement (Elliott, 1982; Revelli, 1955, Gordon, 1998). Hindemith (1946), Shanet (1956), Dallin (1966), and Carlsen (1965) all agreed that learner should tap or walk the underlying beat before learning to read music. Even Cooper (1964) concerned more on reading of pitch than rhythm, advocates a technique of using body movement to learn rhythm. Emile Jaques-Dalcroze eurhythmics teaching method advocates the use of bodily movement to learn underlying and rhythm. Dalcroze believes that the understanding of beat and rhythm is best to learn and absorb through physically feeling it. Dalcroze eurhythmics teaching method is popularly used worldwide, particularly in the field of primary music education (Bachmann, 1991). This method (rhythm and beat learn through bodily movement) received high acclaim and many researches have shown that it is an effective method to teach rhythm and beat (Elliott, 1982; Revelli, 1955, Gordon, 1998).
Rhythmic Patterns
Learning a variety of rhythmic patterns (through tapping, clapping, stepping, playing and saying it (e.g. ta-ti-ta, da-ra-da, etc.) improves the achievement of sight-reading rhythm and hence improves the overall achievement of sight-reading (Keilmann,