Moral Development
Essay Preview: Moral Development
Report this essay
Moral Development
According to Life Span (2006), moral development requires a complex interweaving of emotions, cognitions, and behaviors (Broderick & Blewitt, 2006, p. 221). There are two major theories of moral development: Piagets and Kohlbergs. These two are similar in that they are both stage theories related to cognitive development, but Kohlberg sees moral development as a more complex and longer process than Piagets theory.
Piagets two-stage model proposes a premoral period where preschool children are indifferent about rules and make them up as they go along. At about age 5, Piaget described childrens mortality as heteronomous, where rules are outside the childs sphere and need to be followed exactly, as if the rules were handed down by God, and misbehavior will eventually be punished. In addition, Piaget proposed that the older childs view is more autonomous. The older child understands that it is permissible to change rules if everyone agrees. Rules are not sacred and absolute but are devices which humans use to get along cooperatively (Broderick & Blewitt, 2006, p. 221). Piagets emphasis on egocentrism led him to believe children “project their own thoughts and wishes onto others” and this “uni-directional view of rules and power is associated with objective responsibility” which gives the child confidence in over-evaluating exact rules over reasoning the purpose of the rule (Crain, 1985).
Kohlberg modified and expanded on Piagets work, and focused on late childhood into the adolescent and early adult years (Broderick & Blewitt, 2006, p. 221). Kohlbergs six-stage model includes three levels. The first level is preconventional mortality, which compares to Piagets heteronomous level. The first stage of preconventional mortality has an orientation, of “punishment and obedience” where rules are correct because an authority figure is assumed to be “right.” The second stage of this level has an orientation of concrete/individualistic;” rules are self-serving, but they also will follow the principle of “fair exchange,” such as “You scratch my back, I scratch yours” (Broderick & Blewitt, 2006, p. 222). The second level is conventional and includes a “social-relational perspective” stage, which is the third stage of the six-stage theory. The “social-relational perspective” stage is when the group feelings and needs take on more importance than individual interests. The fourth stage is the “member-of society perspective,” where maintaining the common good of all is considered most important (e.g., obeying laws, and hard work) (Broderick & Blewitt, 2006, p. 222). The postconventional level includes stage five, “prior rights and social contract,” where social rules are valued over specific laws. However, the processes that the specific laws serve are valued (Broderick & Blewitt, 2006, p. 222). The sixth stage of Kohlbergs theory is the “universal ethical principle.” It is considered the final stage where social order is maintained, however principles outweigh exacting laws (Broderick & Blewitt, 2006, p. 222). Kohlberg did not believe the stages were a product of maturation or socialization, rather he thought they occurred by thinking about moral issues (Crain, 1985).
Piagets heteronomous morality stage compares with Kohlbergs punishment and obedience orientation, which highlights obedience of authority figures and literalness of the rules to avoid punishment. Piagets autonomous morality compares with Kohlbergs concrete, individualistic orientation, which highlights the self-serving component of thinking (Broderick & Blewitt, 2006, p. 222). Morals often advanced through “perspective taking,” peer interrelations, and structuring by adults where without these advances, the advance of morality principles might be corralled (p. 223). Broderick and Blewitt limit the usefulness of both Piaget and Kohlberg by stating: (1) Piagets theories abide by a lesser capacity for moral reasoning than is evidenced by studies; (2) Piagets theories do not contain sufficient clarification about how children perceive rules and standards and the degree of authority figure influence; and (3) Piaget does not address cultural, family, or individual discrepancies relating to standards, or in consideration of differences due to moral, conventional, and personal rules (Broderick & Blewitt, 2006, p. 225). The authors consider Kohlbergs theory is limited due to: (1) Kohlbergs legalistic moral dilemmas and his approach to scoring peoples reasoning appears to be gender biased; (2) Kohlbergs theory assumes preschoolers justify sharing and distributive justice only out of concern of punishment, instead of fairness and inclusion; and (3) the reality of moral reasoning is much more complex than Kohlbergs model suggests (Broderick & Blewitt, 2006, pp. 225-226).
Prosocial behavior is defined by the benefit it serves to others and is influenced by emotions, cognitive contributions, temperament and personality, and parents (Broderick & Blewitt, 2006, p. 227). The emotions include empathy and sympathy where the child “feels for another,” though empathy also includes sharing deep-heartedly the feelings of the other (Broderick & Blewitt, 2006, p 240). Cognitive contributions are discussed as either hedonistic where prosocial behavior benefits the benefactor and needs oriented where the other persons needs are the priority (Broderick & Blewitt, 2006, p. 227). Needs orientation can be further considered as socially approved, duty-bound, or owed out of self-respect and values and can be affected by amount of self-sacrifice necessary (Broderick & Blewitt, 2006, pp. 227-228). Temperance and personality are considered flowing evenly through the individuals lifespan and “socially competent,” popular children, children “with a positive global self-concept,” and assertive children appear to show more prosocial behavior than others. Parents, who are authoritative, have prosocial values, model well, and give opportunities to children, are more likely to assist in the childs